Children’s Inborn Belief in Immortality
This article is a brief but fascinating review of research on young children and their intuitive understanding of immortality. Apparently, regardless of their backgrounds, young children are aware of their pre-conception existence. Not surprisingly, what children report about life before conception is not at all similar to the findings of Michael Newton, author of many books on “life between life,” who many consider to be the expert on pre-conception existence. But Newton’s ideas of what happens before birth as well as reincarnation itself are, in my opinion, problematic from many standpoints. I promise to one day post a blog that addresses his work.
What is surprising though is that the researcher of the universal belief in survival after death, whose findings are given below, still holds the unfortunate faux scientific belief that the mind is only a by-product of the brain! I and many others have written forcefully to the contrary. There is by now a great deal of evidence that the mind is independent of the brain. The brain is only a processor.
The following article appeared at http://www.futurity.org/hardwired-believe-immortal. The original study can be found at Whiley Online Library.
ARE WE HARDWIRED TO BELIEVE WE’RE IMMORTAL?
(BOSTON UNIVERSITYright Original StudyPosted by Barbara Moran-Boston University on January 29, 2014)
Most people, regardless of race, religion, or culture, believe they are immortal. That is, people believe that part of themselves—some indelible core, soul, or essence—will live forever.
Why is this belief so unshakable?
A new study published in the journal Child Development sheds light on these profound questions by examining children’s ideas about “prelife,” the time before conception.
Based on interviews with 283 children from two distinct cultures in Ecuador, the researchers say the findings suggest that our bias toward immortality is a part of human intuition that naturally emerges early in life.
And the part of us that is eternal, we believe, is not our skills or ability to reason, but rather our hopes, desires, and emotions. We are, in fact, what we feel.
RELIGION AND SCIENCE
The study led by Natalie Emmons, a postdoctoral fellow at Boston University fits into a growing body of work examining the cognitive roots of religion. Although religion is a dominant force across cultures, science has made little headway in examining whether religious belief—such as the human tendency to believe in a creator—may actually be hardwired into our brains.
“This work shows that it’s possible for science to study religious belief,” says Deborah Kelemen, associate professor of psychology and co-author of the paper. “At the same time, it helps us understand some universal aspects of human cognition and the structure of the mind.”
Most studies on immortality or “eternalist” beliefs have focused on people’s views of the afterlife. Studies have found that both children and adults believe that bodily needs, such as hunger and thirst, end when people die, but mental capacities, such as thinking or feeling sad, continue in some form.
IS IT INTUITION?
But these afterlife studies leave one critical question unanswered: where do these beliefs come from? Researchers have long suspected that people develop ideas about the afterlife through cultural exposure, like television or movies, or through religious instruction.
Emmons thought perhaps these ideas of immortality actually emerge from our intuition. Just as children learn to talk without formal instruction, maybe they also intuit that part of their mind could exist apart from their body.
Emmons tackled this question by focusing on “prelife,” the period before conception, since few cultures have beliefs or views on the subject.
“By focusing on prelife, we could see if culture causes these beliefs to appear, or if they appear spontaneously,” says Emmons.
“I think it’s a brilliant idea,” says Paul Bloom, a professor of psychology and cognitive science at Yale University who was not involved with the study. “One persistent belief is that children learn these ideas through school or church. That’s what makes the prelife research so cool. It’s a very clever way to get at children’s beliefs on a topic where they aren’t given answers ahead of time.”
CHILDREN IN ECUADOR
Emmons interviewed children from an indigenous Shuar village in the Amazon Basin of Ecuador. She chose the group because they have no cultural prelife beliefs, and she suspected that indigenous children, who have regular exposure to birth and death through hunting and farming, would have a more rational, biologically based view of the time before they were conceived.
For comparison, she also interviewed children from an urban area near Quito, Ecuador. Most of the urban children were Roman Catholic, a religion that teaches that life begins only at conception. If cultural influences were paramount, reasoned Emmons, both urban and indigenous children should reject the idea of life before birth.
Emmons showed the children drawings of a baby, a young woman, and the same woman while pregnant, then asked a series of questions about the child’s abilities, thoughts and emotions during each period: as babies, in the womb, and before conception.
The results were surprising. Both groups gave remarkably similar answers, despite their radically different cultures. The children reasoned that their bodies didn’t exist before birth, and that they didn’t have the ability to think or remember.
However, both groups also said that their emotions and desires existed before they were born.
For example, while children generally reported that they didn’t have eyes and couldn’t see things before birth, they often reported being happy that they would soon meet their mother, or sad that they were apart from their family.
“They didn’t even realize they were contradicting themselves,” says Emmons. “Even kids who had biological knowledge about reproduction still seemed to think that they had existed in some sort of eternal form. And that form really seemed to be about emotions and desires.”
FEELINGS ARE FOREVER
Why would humans have evolved this seemingly universal belief in the eternal existence of our emotions? Emmons says that this human trait might be a by-product of our highly developed social reasoning.
“We’re really good at figuring out what people are thinking, what their emotions are, what their desires are,” she says. We tend to see people as the sum of their mental states, and desires and emotions may be particularly helpful when predicting their behavior.
Because this ability is so useful and so powerful, it flows over into other parts of our thinking. We sometimes see connections where potentially none exist, we hope there’s a master plan for the universe, we see purpose when there is none, and we imagine that a soul survives without a body.
These ideas, while nonscientific, are natural and deep-seated.
“I study these things for a living but even find myself defaulting to them. I know that my mind is a product of my brain but I still like to think of myself as something independent of my body,” says Emmons.
“We have the ability to reflect and reason scientifically, and we have the ability to reason based on our gut and intuition,” she adds. “And depending on the situation, one may be more useful than the other.”
Source: Boston University